April 2019
- The human eye has three light-absorbing proteins, which are housed in three different kinds of cone cells
- These three cones produce the sensations of red, green, and blue. In combination, this produces the entire visual range of color that humans can experience.
I’m saying a lot of things we already know, but bear with me! You’d expect then that the light-absorbing abilities of the three red/green/blue proteins in your retina would mirror the LED screen – narrow absorbance centered firmly on the wavelength band which corresponds to the correct color. But let’s take another look at the graph. Peak absorbance for blue is actually deep into the color I would consider violet. And… and… what the heck is up with red?
Peak “red” in the human eye is actually yellowish green.
Notably I have no idea how normalized this graph is, or whether the peak intensity is equivalent between the proteins. Maybe “blue” only weakly responds to light compared to green and “red”. But I still think this says some profound things about the amount of post-processing that has to go on after an image is taken from the retina, particularly if you think about how vividly people experience red, vs blue. I mean, we might have expected this; you have an entire lobe devoted to vision. The backmost chunk of brainmeat is devoted to working out how to see.
Red detection is important in an evolutionary-history sense, since red is the color of blood, meat, and sometimes fertile sex partners (maybe). Three of the four F’s are represented here (fight, flight, food, processes conducive to the propagation of the species).
So what’s going on here? Reds are superenhanced in the optical processing? Maybe, maybe not. “Red” is the only cone cell type delivering graded potentials to your optic nerve during the experience of reddest red.
At 600 nm, green has a relative absorbance of about 2.5, and red at 7, a ratio of about .4. The ratio of green activity to red activity continues to increase – at yellow-green, the ratio is closer to .9.
It is difficult to discriminate a slightly increased firing rate from a single sensory channel (say, red), especially since, unlike a computer, your brain does not have a system clock (more on this in a future post). Perhaps the increased overlap of red and green color fields is adaptive. Perhaps the high clarity of our red visual detection compared to purple or blue is hard-coded into our retinas, and perhaps it is the result of selective post-processing further into the brain. Perhaps both.
More musings on the biology of color detection, v.s. other, learned or post-processing factors.
--- Elizabeth Broadwell
Elizabeth Broadwell is a Masters studnet in the College of Arts and Sciences